close
close

Video shows first clear evidence of narwhals using tusks to play—and stun prey

Narwhals

The Arctic’s iconic narwhal, renowned for its long, spiral tusk, is one of nature’s most fascinating creatures. Yet, few have witnessed how these elusive animals use their tusks in the wild. (Credit: O’Corry-Crowe, FAU/Watt, DFO)

In a nutshell

  • Scientists have discovered narwhals use their tusks for three main purposes: hunting fish with precision strikes, exploring objects with gentle taps, and engaging in what appears to be play behavior.
  • Using drone technology, researchers captured the first-ever footage of narwhals in the wild using their tusks to stun prey, manipulate objects, and interact with each other.
  • These findings challenge the long-held belief that narwhal tusks primarily serve for mating displays, revealing they are versatile tools that may help these animals adapt to a changing Arctic environment.

BOCA RATON, Fla. — The narwhal’s spiral tusk has puzzled scientists and inspired myths for centuries, but until recently, scientists had limited direct observations of how these mysterious Arctic whales use their signature feature in the wild. That changed when researchers flying drones over Canada’s High Arctic captured remarkable footage of narwhals using their tusks as multipurpose tools—precisely tracking and stunning fish, interacting with prey through gentle taps and rotations, and engaging in what appears to be the first documented instance of exploratory-object play in these enigmatic creatures.

The discovery provides scientists with extraordinary new insights into one of nature’s most distinctive adaptations.

A Look Into Narwhals’ Lives Using Drone Technology

The study, published in Frontiers in Marine Science, provides unprecedented visual evidence of tusk use in narwhals. By deploying drones over Creswell Bay, Somerset Island, in the Canadian High Arctic, scientists recorded the whales exhibiting a range of complex behaviors that challenge previous assumptions about the function of their tusks. The footage captured narwhals maneuvering their tusks with remarkable precision, suggesting they use them for much more than display or competition.

The research team, led by Greg O’Corry-Crowe from Florida Atlantic University’s Harbor Branch Oceanographic Institute, collaborated with Fisheries and Oceans Canada and other institutions to analyze the footage. Their findings indicate that tusks are not merely ornamental but serve practical roles in foraging, exploration, and possibly even social interaction.

“Narwhals are known for their ‘tusking’ behavior, where two or more of them simultaneously raise their tusks almost vertically out of the water, crossing them in what may be a ritualistic behavior to assess a potential opponent’s qualities or to display those qualities to potential mates,” said O’Corry-Crowe. “But now we know that narwhal tusks have other uses, some quite unexpected, including foraging, exploration, and likely play.”

The Arctic's iconic narwhal.The Arctic's iconic narwhal.
The Arctic’s iconic narwhal. (Credit: O’Corry-Crowe, FAU/Watt, DFO)

From Curious Investigation to Active Hunting

The researchers documented two distinct behavioral sequences involving multiple narwhals, Arctic char, and in one case, glaucous gulls. These observations revealed fascinating insights into narwhal behavior, including a possible example of exploratory-object play.

In the first sequence, an adult narwhal pursued a single large Arctic char, using the tip of its tusk to closely track the fish’s movements. Two other narwhals soon joined, forming a coordinated group. The lead narwhal occasionally tapped the fish with its tusk, sometimes applying gentle downward pressure or rotating it, but never with enough force to cause harm. This interaction appeared more investigative than predatory, raising the possibility that the whales were exploring a novel prey item rather than attempting to capture it.

Despite multiple opportunities to seize the fish, none of the narwhals made an attempt to consume it. Even when the fish’s tail briefly brushed against one whale’s snout, the whale did not react with a feeding response.

The second observation, recorded two days later, revealed a stark contrast. This time, three younger male narwhals actively hunted smaller Arctic char, striking them with their tusks in rapid succession. One narwhal delivered five swift blows to a fish, using both the tip and shaft of its tusk, successfully stunning the prey.

Seagull Thieves: An Unexpected Complication

This hunting behavior was frequently interrupted by seagulls attempting to steal the narwhals’ prey—a phenomenon known as kleptoparasitism. Opportunistic glaucous gulls swooped down, trying to snatch fish that the whales had targeted.

“Our observations provide clear evidence of narwhals chasing fish and using their tusks to interact directly with the fish and to influence the fish’s behavior,” said O’Corry-Crowe. “Some of the interactions we saw appeared competitive in nature, with one whale blocking or trying to block another whale’s access to the same target fish, while others may have been more subtle, possibly communicative and even affiliative. None appeared overtly aggressive.”

The footage revealed an extraordinary level of dexterity and control in tusk use. Narwhals performed tight, rapid turns—sometimes completing a full 360-degree rotation in under three seconds—to maintain precise positioning. Unlike horizontal turns seen in other marine mammals, narwhals rotated their bodies sideways, bringing their heads downward toward their tails, a movement that appeared to aid in tracking prey with the tip of the tusk.

Play Behavior: A Scientific First?

Perhaps the most intriguing finding was evidence suggesting play behavior. Play is rarely documented in wild animals, particularly adults, because it seemingly offers no immediate survival benefit. However, play is thought to help develop skills, explore novel situations, and strengthen social bonds.

The narwhals’ interaction with the large Arctic char in the first observation met several key criteria for play: it lacked an obvious functional purpose, appeared spontaneous and voluntary, involved exaggerated and repeated actions, and occurred in the absence of stress.

“I have been studying narwhal for over a decade and have always marveled at their tusks,” said Cortney Watt, Ph.D., co-author and research scientist at Fisheries and Oceans, Canada. “To observe them using their tusks for foraging and play is remarkable. This unique study where we set up a remote field camp and spent time filming narwhal with drones is yielding many interesting insights and is providing a bird’s-eye view of their behavior that we have never seen before.”

Pod of narwhals feeding at the surface, Lancaster Sound, Baffin Island, Canada.Pod of narwhals feeding at the surface, Lancaster Sound, Baffin Island, Canada.
Pod of narwhals feeding at the surface, Lancaster Sound, Baffin Island, Canada. (© wildestanimal – stock.adobe.com)

Social Learning in a Changing Arctic

The study also highlighted complex social dynamics among narwhals. In some cases, whales competed for access to fish, blocking one another’s approach. In others, they seemed to cooperate or communicate, adjusting their positions in response to one another’s movements. These interactions suggest narwhals might learn from each other, with younger individuals potentially observing and adopting hunting techniques from more experienced adults.

This behavioral flexibility may play a crucial role in how narwhals adapt to the rapidly changing Arctic environment. Climate change is shifting prey distributions and ice conditions, forcing marine mammals to adjust their foraging strategies. The observed ability of narwhals to experiment with different tusk uses—such as testing new prey species—suggests they may have some capacity to adapt.

“To understand how narwhals are being affected by and adapting to the changing Arctic, field studies using innovative, non-invasive tools like drones are essential to observe them in their natural environment without disturbing them,” said O’Corry-Crowe. “Drones provide a unique, real-time view of their behavior, helping scientists gather crucial data on how narwhals are responding to shifts in ice patterns, prey availability, and other environmental changes.”

The narwhal’s tusk—once the stuff of unicorn legends—turns out to be not just an ornament for attracting mates or intimidating rivals, but a tool that helps these marine beauties navigate their icy world, hunt their prey, and perhaps even have a little fun along the way.

Paper Summary

Methodology

The research team conducted their study in Creswell Bay, Somerset Island, in the Canadian High Arctic during the summer of 2022. They used unmanned aerial vehicles (UAVs)—specifically a DJI Phantom 4 PRO+ V2.0 drone—to observe and record narwhal behaviors. On August 4, they launched the drone from shore and flew it at various altitudes (all at least 20 meters above the water) over a dispersed group of narwhals that had entered the inner bay. Two days later, on August 6, they flew the drone over a more compact group of narwhals near the mouth of the Kuksik River in the inner bay.

The researchers analyzed the video footage using specialized software, including DAVINCI RESOLVE, CAPCUT, and Movavi, to identify and describe discrete behaviors. They slowed the video to between 0.1 and 0.5 times normal speed and zoomed in to assist with detection and description of specific actions. They categorized behaviors as either “state event behaviors” (those with a time duration of more than 5 seconds) or “point event behaviors” (brief actions lasting 5 seconds or less). The research team developed an ethogram—a catalog of behaviors—and used event-logging software called BORIS to construct ethograms for each individual animal (whale, fish, gull) and analyze patterns of observed behaviors, including time budgets.

Results

The researchers documented two distinct behavioral sequences involving narwhals, Arctic char, and in one case, glaucous gulls. In the first observation, three narwhals pursued a single large Arctic char for nearly 5 minutes. The lead narwhal hit the fish with its tusk 12 times, with 6 additional possible hits, but these contacts were gentle and primarily appeared exploratory rather than predatory. None of the narwhals attempted to catch or consume the fish.

The second observation, two days later, showed more active hunting behavior. Three narwhals pursued multiple smaller char, with two of the narwhals hitting fish with their tusks using forceful, rapid strikes that stunned the fish. In this observation, glaucous gulls frequently attempted to steal fish the narwhals were pursuing—a behavior known as kleptoparasitism. Statistical analysis revealed significant differences between the two observations, with narwhals in the second observation spending more time searching for prey and less time in pursuit compared to the first observation. They also spent less time associating with each other and exhibited more rapid behavioral transitions. The researchers documented 17 distinct behaviors across both observations, including travel, pursuit, association, searching, evasion, tusk hits, pushing with tusks, and interactions between whales.

Limitations

The research, while groundbreaking, has several limitations. The observations were limited to two brief behavioral sequences during a single summer season, making it difficult to determine how common these behaviors are in narwhal populations more broadly. The UAV observations were also limited to narwhals at or near the surface in clear water, potentially missing behaviors that occur at greater depths. Additionally, while Arctic char were observed in the narwhals’ diet during this study, previous research has found limited evidence of char in the diet of this particular narwhal population, raising questions about whether the observed behaviors represent typical feeding patterns or unusual interactions. The researchers also acknowledge that interpreting animal behavior, particularly assessing intentions or distinguishing between foraging, exploration, and play, involves some subjectivity. Finally, the sample size was small, with observations of only a few individuals, which may not represent the full behavioral repertoire of narwhals.

Discussion and Takeaways

This study provides compelling evidence that narwhals use their tusks for multiple purposes beyond sexual selection, including investigating and manipulating objects, hitting and stunning fish, and possibly engaging in play. The differences between the two observations suggest narwhals may adapt their tusk use based on context, using different techniques for different purposes or prey types. The apparent play behavior documented in the first observation represents the first reported evidence of play in narwhals, specifically what the researchers call “exploratory-object play.”

The social interactions observed among narwhals suggest complex social dynamics that may include competitive behaviors, communication, social learning, and potentially instruction from experienced adults to immature individuals. The kleptoparasitic behavior of gulls represents a previously undocumented interspecific interaction that may significantly affect narwhal foraging success near the surface. From an evolutionary perspective, the study suggests that while sexual selection may have been the primary force driving the evolution of narwhal tusks, these appendages have developed secondary functions similar to the tusks of elephants and walruses. In the context of a rapidly changing Arctic, the observed behavioral flexibility and exploratory behavior may be important for narwhals’ adaptation to new environmental conditions, including potentially encountering new prey species.

Funding and Disclosures

The research was supported by multiple organizations, including Fisheries and Oceans Canada, Harbor Branch Oceanographic Institute at Florida Atlantic University, the World Wildlife Fund, the Nunavut Wildlife Management Board, the National Geographic Society, and Natural Resources Canada’s Polar Continental Shelf Program. The research was conducted under permit A-22/23-002-NU and animal care protocol OPA-ACC-2022-25 issued by Fisheries and Oceans Canada. The authors declared no conflicts of interest, and no generative AI was used in the creation of the original manuscript. The study was conducted in partnership with Inuit communities in Nunavut in Canada’s High Arctic, recognizing the importance of collaborating with local communities in wildlife research.

Publication Information

The study, titled “Use of tusks by narwhals, Monodon monoceros, in foraging, exploratory, and play behavior,” was published in Frontiers in Marine Science on February 28, 2025. The authors are Greg O’Corry-Crowe, Maha Ghazal, Mark Gillespie, Paul Galvin, Jason Harasimo, Luke Storrie, and Cortney A. Watt, representing institutions including Harbor Branch Oceanographic Institute at Florida Atlantic University, Fisheries and Oceans Canada, World Wildlife Fund – Canada, Wildlife Conservation Society Canada, University of Victoria, and University of Manitoba. The paper is available as an open-access article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License.